Alexandre F. (2017) How to Understand Brain-Body-Environment Interactions? Toward a Systemic Representationalism. Constructivist Foundations 13(1): 130–131. https://cepa.info/4415
Open peer commentary on the article “Missing Colors: The Enactivist Approach to Perception” by Adrián G. Palacios, María-José Escobar & Esteban Céspedes. Upshot: The target article discusses the influence of the enactivist account of perception in computer science, beyond subjectivism and objectivism. I suggest going one step further and introduce our VirtualEnaction platform, proposing a federative systemic view for brain-body-environment interaction for this analysis.
Auvray M., Lenay C. & Stewart J. (2009) Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas in Psychology 27: 32–47. https://cepa.info/478
Minimalism is a useful element in the constructivist arsenal against objectivism. By reducing actions and sensory feedback to a bare minimum, it becomes possible to obtain a complete description of the sensory-motor dynamics; and this in turn reveals that the object of perception does not pre-exist in itself, but is actually constituted during the process of observation. In this paper, this minimalist approach is deployed for the case of the recognition of “the Other.” It is shown that the perception of another intentional subject is based on properties that are intrinsic to the joint perceptual activity itself.
Bergman M. (2011) Beyond the Interaction Paradigm? Radical Constructivism, Universal Pragmatics, and Peircean Pragmatism. The Communication Review 14(2): 96–122.
In this article, the author examines Colin Grant’s recent criticism of the so-called “interaction paradigm” and Jürgen Habermas’s universal pragmatics. Grant’s approach, which is presented as an open challenge to communication theories grounded in philosophical conceptions of communality and dialogue, can be construed as an exemplar of a radical constructivist approach to vital questions of contingency and incommensurability in communication studies. In response, the author outlines a classical pragmatist approach to the problem areas identified by Grant, with the aim of outlining how a pragmatist outlook can offer promising theoretical alternatives to universal pragmatics and radical constructivism. It is argued that moderate Peircean pragmatism, appropriately interpreted, can provide a philosophical platform capable of addressing issues of contingency, uncertainty, and autonomy in communication theory without succumbing to incommensurabilism, traditional objectivism, or nominalistic individualism.
Carson J. (2005) Objectivism and education: A response to David Elkind’s “The Problem with Constructivism”. The Educational Forum 69(3): 232–238. https://cepa.info/4588
This paper responds to David Elkind’s article “The Problem with Constructivism, ” published in the Summer 2004issue ofThe Educational Forum. It argues that Elkind’s thesis – teacher, curricular, and societal readiness lead to the implementation of constructivism – is conceptually problematic. This paper also critiques constructivism and supports objectivism as a viable philosophy of education.
Castañon G. (2015) O que é construtivismo? [What is constructivism?]. Cadernos de História e Filosofia da Ciência, Campinas (Série 4) 1(2): 209–242. https://cepa.info/5961
‘Constructivism’ is a term adopted by many contemporary philosophical approaches. It appeared with the work of Piaget, and since then it has been appropriated by many approaches with different ontological and epistemological orientations. This article examines some of these major contemporary appropriations on three issues: the problem of realism and the problems of possibility and source of knowledge. Piagetian constructivism, socioconstructivism, logical constructivism, radical constructivism and social constructivism are analyzed. The purpose of this study || is to provide a better definition of the term, maintaining its link to its historical origin, and at the same time to be helpful in clarifying its indiscriminate use. A part of the conceptual confusion surrounding the term is solved with the dissolution of the false polarities between realism and constructivism, and between objectivism and skepticism. The conclusion is that the positions vary between realism and antirealism, and between criticism and relativism. It finds in the problem of the source of knowledge the common denominators of all allegations of constructivism: the rejection of empiricist objectivism and the adoption of the Kantian sense of the construction metaphor. We can positively define constructivism as the epistemological thesis that supports the active role of the subject in creating and modifying his representations of the object of knowledge.
Cobb T. (2006) Constructivism. In: Brown K. (ed.) Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Second edition. Elsevier, Amsterdam: 85–87. https://cepa.info/4679
Constructivism, the notion that knowledge must be assembled from pieces rather than assimilated whole, has been a principal learning theory in psychology for about 20 years and in psycholinguistics for 10. The theory is now making a strong entry into educational thinking, but in language education it is less in evidence. That is because applied linguists have always been constructivists, implicitly, and have already confronted some of the implementation problems facing constructivism in mathematics or science education. Nevertheless, a more explicit understanding of the constructivist approach is useful within language education, particularly in providing a framework for exploiting information technologies.
Duffy T. M. & Jonassen D. H. (1991) Constructivism: New implications for instructional technology? Educational Technology 31(5): 7–12. https://cepa.info/6736
Excerpt: Constructivism provides an alternative epistemologica! base to the objectivist tradition. Constructivism, like objectivism, holds that there is a real world that we experience. However, the argument is that meaning is imposed on the world by us, rather than existing in the world independently of us. There are many ways to structure the world and there are many meanings or perspectives for any event or concept. Thus, there is not a correct meaning that we are striving for.
Feng Y. (1996) Some thoughts about applying constructivist theories to guide instruction. Computers in the Schools 12(3): 71–84.
Excerpt: Many of us share the conviction that constructivist learning theories have the potential to inspire the development of new models of instructional practice. We also believe that knowing how people learn will influence how we help them learn. However, constructivist learning theories should not, and do not, privilege or endorse any particular mode of instruction. The best way to teach will be one that helps students learn a specific subject or a skill most effectively, whether it be invention, reproduction, replication, memorization, or mimicry. This paper addresses two issues: the basic assumptions and characteristics of constructivist theories of learning (CTL), and the circumstances under which they may be applied to guide instruction. In order to answer questions related to the above issues, it is necessary to clarify the differences between the major concepts of objectivism and constructivism. Also, it is necessary to understand the differences between various schools of CTL before drawing on any of them to guide instruction.
Guidano V. F. (1995) Constructivist psychotherapy: A theoretical framework. In: Neimeyer R. A. & Mahoney M. J. (eds.) Constructivism in psychotherapy. American Psychological Association, Washington DC: 93–108.
[Offers an understanding] about how people develop and how a constructivist (developmental process-oriented) psychotherapist might conceptualize and facilitate such development / [argues that] when reality is assumed to be an objective external order that exists independently from peoples’ observations of it – an assumption common to objectivism, realism, and traditional rationalism – it is inevitable that people will overlook their own characteristics and processes as observers / from [a constructivist] nonobjectivist perspective, an essential task becomes understanding how people’s characteristics as observers are involved in the process of observing, as well as how people otherwise participate in cocreating the dynamic personal realities to which they individually respond / this shift leads necessarily to a radical change in traditional formulations of human experience, human knowing, and professional helping outline here some of the basic features inherent to the nature and structure of human experience, with the aim of deriving from them a consistent methodology and strategy for cognitive therapy
Hoops W. (1998) Konstruktivismus: Ein neues Paradigma für Didaktisches Design? Unterrichtswissenschaft 26(3): 229–253. https://cepa.info/3727
Abstract: It is trendy to be a “ constructivist,” although this designation is not very illuminating, since very divergent positions are subsumed under this lobe!. In order to arrive at a more precise idea of what constructivism is or might be, exemplary text passages taken from the discussion of radical constructivism in the United States in the past few years are analyzed. Against the background of two contrasting epistemological positions (“objectivism and “ constructivism”), constructivism in the field of education defines itself by its reference to constructivist epistemology and by the assumption that instructional design can be deduced, as it were, from an epistemology. This “ deduction assumption,” though, leads to a problematic confounding of epistemological and pedagogical perspectives – this is especially true of the central concept of “construction” – and to an apparent antithesis of “ objectivistic” and “constructivistic” instructional design. Constructivism’s dichotomic approach permeates all spheres of instructional design and tends to lead to a lack of necessary differentiation and to obscure relevant pedagogic problems and contexts. Therefore, it is proposed to dispense with the concept of “constructivism” in the pedagogic field altogether and to join instead Greeno’s search for a synthesis of cognitive psychology and situated learning.