Toggle navigation
CEPA.INFO
FAQ
BROWSE
Authors
Constructivist Approaches
Background Disciplines
Reading Lists
Latest Fulltext Additions
LOGIN
Search Results
Publications Found:
143
·
Show All Abstracts
·
Highlight Matches
Search CEPA
» Help with Search
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x2f���������������������������������������������������������������������������������B
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x2f������������������������������������������������������������������������������
fulltext:"Man, having within himself an imagined world of lines and numbers, operates in it with abstractions just as God in the universe, did with reality"
fulltext:"Man, having within himself an imagined world of lines and numbers, operates in it with abstractions just as God in the universe, did with reality"
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????? ORDER BY 3989
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/?????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????) ORDER BY 7543
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/?????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????' ORDER BY 4180
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/?????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????') ORDER BY 4953
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/?????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????' ORDER BY 7206
fulltext:maturana9999922unionselectunhex(hex(version()))--22x22=22x/????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????? ORDER BY 8748
By default, Find returns all publications that contain the words in the surnames of their author, in their titles, or in their years. For example,
Maturana
finds all publications authored by Maturana and publications that have "Maturana" in their title
Maturana 1974
finds all publications authored by Maturana in 1974
You can directly search for a reference by copy-pasting it. For example,
Glasersfeld E. von (1974) Jean Piaget and the radical constructivist epistemology
Unless a word (or phrase) if prefixed with a minus (-) it must be present in all results. Examples:
Glasersfeld Varela
shows all publications Ernst von Glasersfeld and Francisco Varela wrote together.
Glasersfeld "Jean Piaget"
finds all publications with
Glasersfeld
and
Jean Piaget
in it.
Prefix with
-
to indicate that this word must not be present in any result:
cognition -biology
will find entries that have
cognition
in the title but not
biology
.
Enter the surname of an author and a year to find all publications the author wrote in that year:
Glasersfeld 1995
presents all publications Ernst von Glasersfeld published in 1995.
Use
*
to match any characters:
constructivis*
matches constructivism and constructivist.
Enclose phrases between double quotes
"
to force phrase search:
"biology of cognition"
lists only the publications containing this phrase. Without the double quotes it will return all publications containing "biology" and all publications containing "cognition".
All the searches above match author names, titles and years. You can also address single fields:
author:glasersfeld title:reality
shows publications von Glasersfeld wrote on reality;
abstract:second-order
searches all abstracts for "second-order";
editor:Watzlawick
finds all books edited by Watzlawick.
Note there is no space after the colon.
Attention: Words of three letters and less are ignored.
"Not one, not two"
will return no result although there is
Varela's paper
of this title.
Adams P. (2006) Exploring social constructivism: Theories and practicalities. Education 34(3): 3–13. https://cepa.info/3958
Adams P.
(
2006
)
Exploring social constructivism: Theories and practicalities
.
Education
34(3): 3–13.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/3958
Copy Citation
In the drive to improve standards, the collection and dissemination of numerical data still directs much contemporary educational policy. However, recent publications and debates seemingly attempt to reorient discussion from performance to learning. In support, constructivism is often referenced as a contributor in this endeavour. However, constructivism is not a single unified theory either of knowledge or pedagogy. This article identifies one version of constructivist thinking, social constructivism, both in terms of its underlying epistemology (theory of knowledge) and related pedagogy. Contemporary educational theories are then outlined to demonstrate that many practical solutions and theoretical ideas now presented as ‘good learning and teaching’ have much in common with social constructivist thinking. Finally, the article concludes by identifying two issues that require further discussion and debate if pedagogy of a social constructivist nature is to be considered.
Amamou Y. & Stewart J. (2006) Analyse descriptive de trajectoires perceptives [Representations: External memory and technical artefacts]. In: Proceedings of the 18th Conference on l’Interaction Homme-Machine (IHM’06), Montréal, Canada, 18–21 April 2006. ACM Press, New York: 145–148.
Amamou Y.
&
Stewart J.
(
2006
)
Analyse descriptive de trajectoires perceptives
[Representations: External memory and technical artefacts].
In:
Proceedings of the 18th Conference on l’Interaction Homme-Machine (IHM’06), Montréal, Canada, 18–21 April
2006
. ACM Press, New York: 145–148.
Copy Citation
We wished in this present study, to describe and automate the identification of the strategies implemented by blindfolded subjects. we had resource to the transformation of Fourier in our description. The transform of Fourier is known by his historical relevance in the characterization of the rhythmic behaviour. The trajectories of the subjects are recorded by means of substitution tactile interface. We could define descriptors that differentiate the perceptive strategies. deployed by the subjects. This study has goal to facilitate the acquistion of sensory substitution device. by offering to the subjects rules and strategies which they can adopt. thus they reduce the period of training phase.
Amamou Y. & Stewart J. (2006) Perceptive strategies with an enactive interface. In: ENACTIVE/06: Enaction & Complexity. Proceedings of the 3rd international conference on enactive interfaces. Association ACROE, Grenoble: 101–102. https://cepa.info/7200
Amamou Y.
&
Stewart J.
(
2006
)
Perceptive strategies with an enactive interface
.
In:
ENACTIVE/06: Enaction & Complexity. Proceedings of the 3rd international conference on enactive interfaces
. Association ACROE, Grenoble: 101–102.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/7200
Copy Citation
This study reveals that when human subjects use an “enactive interface” to perceive graphic forms, they employ a variety of perceptive strategies. The aim of the present paper is to introduce the question of the significance of the enaction concept in perceptive strategies of graphic forms.
Arnoldi J. (2006) Autopoiesis. Theory, Culture & Society 23(2–3): 116–117. https://cepa.info/6282
Arnoldi J.
(
2006
)
Autopoiesis
.
Theory, Culture & Society
23(2–3): 116–117.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/6282
Copy Citation
Key words:
autopoiesis
,
complexity theory
,
cybernetics
,
luhmann
Asaro P. M. (2006) Computers as models of the mind: On simulations, brains and the design of early computers. In: Franchi S. & Bianchini F. (eds.) The search for a theory of cognition: Early mechanisms and new ideas. Rodopi, Amsterdam: 89–116. https://cepa.info/5026
Asaro P. M.
(
2006
)
Computers as models of the mind: On simulations, brains and the design of early computers
.
In: Franchi S. & Bianchini F. (eds.)
The search for a theory of cognition: Early mechanisms and new ideas
. Rodopi, Amsterdam: 89–116.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/5026
Copy Citation
Excerpt:
The purpose of this essay is to clarify some of the important senses in which the relationship between the brain and the computer might be considered as one of “modeling.” It also considers the meaning of “simulation” in the relationships between models, computers and brains. While there has been a fairly broad literature emerging on models and simulations in science, these have primarily focused on the physical sciences, rather than the mind and brain. And while the cognitive sciences have often invoked concepts of modeling and simulation, they have been frustratingly inconsistent in their use of these terms, and the implicit relations to their scientific roles. My approach is to consider the early convolution of brain models and computational models in cybernetics, with the aim of clarifying their significance for more current debates in the cognitive sciences. It is my belief that clarifying the historical senses in which the brain and computer serve as models of each other in the historical period prior to the birth of AI and cognitive science is a crucial task for an archeology of AI and the history of cognitive science.
Asaro P. M. (2006) On the origins of the synthetic mind: Working models, mechanisms, and simulations. . https://cepa.info/4732
Asaro P. M.
(
2006
)
On the origins of the synthetic mind: Working models, mechanisms, and simulations
.
.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/4732
Copy Citation
This dissertation reconsiders the nature of scientific models through an historical study of the development of electronic models of the brain by Cybernetics researchers in the 1940s. By examining how these unique models were used in the brain sciences, it develops the concept of a “working model” for the brain sciences. Working models differ from theoretical models in that they are subject to manipulation and interactive experimentation, i.e., they are themselves objects of study and part of material culture. While these electronic brains are often disparaged by historians as toys and publicity stunts, I argue that they mediated between physiological theories of neurons and psychological theories of behavior so as to leverage their compelling material performances against the lack of observational data and sparse theoretical connections between neurology and psychology. I further argue that working models might be used by cognitive science to better understand how the brain develops performative representations of the world.
Baecker D. (2006) Niklas Luhmann in the Society of the Computer. Cybernetics & Human Knowing 13(2): 25–40. https://cepa.info/3327
Baecker D.
(
2006
)
Niklas Luhmann in the Society of the Computer
.
Cybernetics & Human Knowing
13(2): 25–40.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/3327
Copy Citation
Niklas Luhmann is not exactly known for his thinking about a possible change of the society due to the introduction of the computer. His society is the modern society, based on the overall importance of the communication medium of the printing press. Yet, his double volume book on Die Gesellschaft der Gesellschaft is so rich in remarks about the possible influence of the introduction of the computer on the society, equal only to the introduction of, first, writing and, then, the printing press, that one might be tempted to consider this book his way to bid farewell to the modern culture of the society based on the printing press. Let us look at what modern society has achieved relying on a notion of order stemming, with only slight exaggeration, from the library, and then let us try to watch how this very same society has to find equally wide-ranging solutions to a society relying, for a dominant part of its communication, on an order adapted to the computing machine, or so he seems to tell us. This paper looks at Die Gesellschaft der Gesellschaft in terms of a theory of the emerging computer culture of a society we cannot any more call the modern one. And it proposes to call for a competition to complete one of the most speculative chapters of this book in which Luhmann attributes the central cultural notion, or theory form, of the literal society, telos, to Aristotle, of the printing press society, self-referential restlessness, to Descartes, and leaves the slot open for the one possibly defining the culture of the computer society, which is the theory form of the form.
Barandiaran X. & Moreno A. (2006) On what makes certain dynamical systems cognitive: A minimally cognitive organization program. Adaptive Behavior 14(2): 171–185. https://cepa.info/4513
Barandiaran X.
&
Moreno A.
(
2006
)
On what makes certain dynamical systems cognitive: A minimally cognitive organization program
.
Adaptive Behavior
14(2): 171–185.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/4513
Copy Citation
Dynamicism has provided cognitive science with important tools to understand some aspects of “how cognitive agents work” but the issue of “what makes something cognitive” has not been sufficiently addressed yet and, we argue, the former will never be complete without the latter. Behavioristic characterizations of cognitive properties are criticized in favor of an organizational approach focused on the internal dynamic relationships that constitute cognitive systems. A definition of cognition as adaptive-autonomy in the embodied and situated neurodynamic domain is provided: the compensatory regulation of a web of stability dependencies between sensorimotor structures is created and pre served during a historical/developmental process. We highlight the functional role of emotional embodiment: internal bioregulatory processes coupled to the formation and adaptive regulation of neurodynamic autonomy. Finally, we discuss a “minimally cognitive behavior program” in evolutionary simulation modeling suggesting that much is to be learned from a complementary “minimally cognitive organization program”
Key words:
minimal cognition
,
adaptive autonomy
,
neurodynamic organization
,
cognition-as-it-could-be
,
emotional embodiment
Barandiaran X. E. & Moreno A. (2006) ALife models as epistemic artefacts. In: Rocha L., Yaeger L., Bedau M., Floreano D., Goldstone R. & Vespignani A. (eds.) Artificial life X.: Proceedings of the tenth international conference on the simulation and synthesis of living systems. MIT Press, Cambridge: 513–519.
Barandiaran X. E.
&
Moreno A.
(
2006
)
ALife models as epistemic artefacts
.
In: Rocha L., Yaeger L., Bedau M., Floreano D., Goldstone R. & Vespignani A. (eds.)
Artificial life X.: Proceedings of the tenth international conference on the simulation and synthesis of living systems
. MIT Press, Cambridge: 513–519.
Copy Citation
Bich L. (2006) Autopoiesis and emergence. In: Minati G., Pessa E. & Abram M. (eds.) Systemics of emergence: Research and development. Springer, Berlin: 281–292. https://cepa.info/2320
Bich L.
(
2006
)
Autopoiesis and emergence
.
In: Minati G., Pessa E. & Abram M. (eds.)
Systemics of emergence: Research and development
. Springer, Berlin: 281–292.
Fulltext at https://cepa.info/2320
Copy Citation
Autopoietic theory is more than a mere characterization of the living, as it can be applied to a wider class of systems and involves both organizational and epistemological aspects. In this paper we assert the necessity of considering the relation between autopoiesis and emergence, focusing on the crucial importance of the observer’s activity and demonstrating that autopoietic systems can be considered intrinsically emergent processes. From the attempts to conceptualize emergence, especially Rosen’s, autopoiesis stands out for its attention to the unitary character of systems and to emergent levels, both inseparable from the observer’s operations. These aspects are the basis of Varela’s approach to multiple level relationships, considered as descriptive complementarities.
Key words:
Autopoiesis
,
emergence
,
observer
,
descriptive complementarity
,
Robert Rosen
,
Francisco Varela
Export result page as:
CF Format
·
APA
·
BibTex
·
EndNote
·
Harvard
·
MLA
·
Nature
·
RIS
·
Science
Page
1
2
...
14
15
Please provide us with your
feedback/evaluation/suggestions